“The only way for interaction with Iran is dialogue on an equal footing, confidence-building and mutual respect as well as reducing antagonism and aggression,” Iranian President Hassan Rouhani said in a speech after taking the oath of office last August.
“If you want the right response, don’t speak with Iran in the language of sanctions, speak in the language of respect.”
In the following article, Nicholas Wright and Karim Sadjadpour describe how an understanding of neuroscience — or lack of it — may determine the outcome of negotiations with Iran.
The whole piece is worth reading, but keep this in mind: every single insight that gets attributed to neuroscience has been clearly established without the need to conduct a single brain scan. Indeed, everything that is here being attributed to the “exquisite neural machinery” of the brain can be understood by studying the workings of the human mind and how thought shapes behavior.
It is important to draw a sharp distinction between the examination of the mind and observing the workings of the brain because the latter is totally dependent on the output of intermediary electronic scanning devices, whereas minds can study themselves and each other directly and through shared language.
One of the insidious effects of neuroscience is that it promotes a view that understanding the ways brains work has greater intrinsic value than understanding how minds work. What the negotiations with Iran demonstrate, however, is that the exact opposite is true.
To the extent that through the development of trust, negotiations are able to advance, this will have nothing to do with anyone’s confidence about what is happening inside anyone’s brain. On the contrary, it will depend on a meeting of minds and mutual understanding. No one will need to understand what is happening in their own or anyone else’s insula cortex, but what will most likely make or break the talks will be whether the Iranians believe they are being treated fairly. The determination of fairness does not depend on the presence or absence of a particular configuration of neural activity but rather on an assessment of reality.
Treat us as equals, Iran’s president said — and that was almost 15 years ago!
Nicholas Wright and Karim Sadjadpour write: “Imagine being told that you cannot do what everyone else is doing,” appealed Iranian Foreign Minister Javad Zarif in a somber YouTube message defending the country’s nuclear program in November. “Would you back down? Would you relent? Or would you stand your ground?”
While only 14 nations, including Iran, enrich uranium (e.g. “what everyone else is doing”), Zarif’s message raises a question at the heart of ongoing talks to implement a final nuclear settlement with Tehran: Why has the Iranian government subjected its population to the most onerous sanctions regime in contemporary history in order to do this? Indeed, it’s estimated that Iran’s antiquated nuclear program needs one year to enrich as much uranium as Europe’s top facility produces in five hours.
To many, the answer is obvious: Iran is seeking a nuclear weapons capability (which it has arguably already attained), if not nuclear weapons. Yet the numerous frameworks used to explain Iranian motivations—including geopolitics, ideology, nationalism, domestic politics, and threat perception—lead analysts to different conclusions. Does Iran want nuclear weapons to dominate the Middle East, or does it simply want the option to defend itself from hostile opponents both near and far? While there’s no single explanation for Tehran’s actions, if there is a common thread that connects these frameworks and may help illuminate Iranian thinking, it is the brain.
Although neuroscience can’t be divorced from culture, history, and geography, there is no Orientalism of the brain: The fundamental biology of social motivations is the same in Tokyo, Tehran, and Tennessee. It anticipates, for instance, how the mind’s natural instinct to reject perceived unfairness can impede similarly innate desires for accommodation, and how fairness can lead to tragedy. It tells us that genuinely conciliatory gestures are more likely and natural than many believe, and how to make our own conciliatory gestures more effective.
Distilled to their essence, nations are led by and comprised of humans, and the success of social animals like humans rests on our ability to control the balance between cooperation and self-interest. The following four lessons from neuroscience may help us understand the obstacles that were surmounted to reach an interim nuclear deal with Iran, and the enormous challenges that still must be overcome in order to reach a comprehensive agreement. [Continue reading…]
Why has Iran gone nuclear? For the same reason that the US encouraged Iran to go nuclear back in the 1970s: because it makes economic sense and is Iran’s sovereign right. Why is THAT explanation ignored in favor of this nonsense about brains and nukes?
A few days ago in an op-ed in the New York Times, Siegfried S. Hecker and William J. Perry wrote:
I’m sure Hecker and Perry believe that their proposals make eminently good sense and their implementation would meet everyone’s best interests. The problem is, they are ignoring the psychological dimension of the issue. Since Iran has already asserted enrichment as a non-negotiable right, it can’t reverse that position without appearing to have capitulated to Western pressure.
The underlying issue has next to nothing to do with nuclear power. It’s really about the West asserting its “right” to impose its authority on the rest of the world. Since this is the psychological subtext, it’s not something whose terms can be explicitly negotiated. But understanding how people deal with issues of fairness, is not a peripheral matter that would only interest casual observers. It’s something about which that the negotiators themselves need to think with the greatest possible clarity.
“Indeed, everything that is here being attributed to the “exquisite neural machinery” of the brain can be understood by studying the workings of the human mind and how thought shapes behavior.”
You’re 100% right on this. Most cognitive scientists would take exactly the same line. Unfortunately, the prefix “neuro” has a lot of pop cred, and, as in any discipline, there are charlatans and quasi-charlatans (as well as people who simply can’t reason outside the very narrow paradigms they were trained in) who are eager to make some kind of public splash, often in aid of selling a book. My most recent favorite in this literature is the OpEd piece in the NYT by the publicity-seeking professor who claimed that neuroscience validated her decision to lie elaborately to her kids about Santa.
In my comments, I’m not attempting to debunk neuroscience altogether. There are lots of ways in which greater understanding of brain functioning can shed new light on human experience, but what I’m arguing against is the reduction of human experience to its neurological correlates.
For instance, a recent study that the brain contains two internal clocks is interesting and, I think, an example of the kind of thing that couldn’t be understood without the use of brain imaging techniques.
But when it comes to issues that matter hugely to human beings individually and collectively — questions such as what causes happiness or makes people selfish or what it means to be authentic — I can think of no inquiring mind throughout history whose insights into these issues would have been more profound had they had access to an fMRI.
Would Shakespeare have been a better playwright if he’d been able to read all the latest studies in the neuroscience journals? Obviously not.
No amount of brain studying will yield truths such as these.
Yes, but behind this lies the very interesting fact that the mind can be, has been, is being, studied scientifically, with the usual effect that good science discloses unimaginably ‘more than is dreamt of in your philosophy’. The same, by the way, is true in animal cognition.
We have to distinguish between real neuroscience, which has many deep & complicated questions to ask at the most fundamental level about how the apparatus works, and the pop variety, which is absurdly reductive and broad-brush — almost a form of attention-seeking.